|
1 | 1 | %% This BibTeX bibliography file was created using BibDesk.
|
2 | 2 | %% http://bibdesk.sourceforge.net/
|
3 | 3 |
|
4 |
| -%% Created for j at 2021-09-10 10:36:49 -0400 |
5 |
| -
|
6 |
| -
|
7 |
| -%% Saved with string encoding Unicode (UTF-8) |
| 4 | +%% Created for j at 2021-09-10 10:36:49 -0400 |
8 | 5 |
|
9 | 6 |
|
| 7 | +%% Saved with string encoding Unicode (UTF-8) |
10 | 8 |
|
11 | 9 | @inproceedings{hoover.j:2020,
|
12 |
| - abstract = {Icelandic dative-nominative constructions exhibit a syntactic hierarchy effect known as the Person Restriction: only third person nominatives may control agreement. In these constructions, there is variation between speakers in the extent to which the verb agrees with the nominative for number. Sigurðsson & Holmberg (2008) explain this variation as arising due to differences between varieties in the timing of subject raising, using a split phi-probe. This paper revises their approach, using the feature gluttony mechanism for Agree developed in Coon & Keine (2020), and a split phi-probe in which person probing precedes number probing. Within this framework, the observed variation can be captured by allowing variability two independent parameters: the timing of EPP subject raising, and the visibility of a number feature on dative DPs. The proposed mechanism describes the variation, including predicting the observed optional agreement in certain cases that previous literature had struggled to account for, and makes additional predictions about the differences between varieties in cases of syncretism within the verbal paradigm. An investigation into these predictions should allow this already well-studied area of Icelandic grammar to continue to be a useful test-case for crosslinguistic assumptions about the mechanism of Agree, and the status of dative arguments.}, |
13 |
| - address = {Somerville, Mass., USA}, |
14 |
| - author = {Jacob Louis Hoover}, |
15 |
| - booktitle = {Proceedings of the 38th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics}, |
16 |
| - editor = {Daniel Reisinger and Rachel Soo}, |
17 |
| - handout = {http://doi.org/10.14288/1.0389856}, |
18 |
| - pdf = {wccfl2020-icelandic_gluttony-proceedings.pdf}, |
19 |
| - publisher = {Cascadilla Proceedings Project}, |
20 |
| - pubstate = {forthcoming}, |
21 |
| - title = {Accounting for variation in number agreement in Icelandic dative--nominative constructions}, |
22 |
| - year = {2020}} |
| 10 | + abstract = {Icelandic dative-nominative constructions exhibit a syntactic |
| 11 | + hierarchy effect known as the Person Restriction: only third |
| 12 | + person nominatives may control agreement. In these constructions, |
| 13 | + there is variation between speakers in the extent to which the |
| 14 | + verb agrees with the nominative for number. <a |
| 15 | + href="https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/000371">Sigurðsson & Holmberg |
| 16 | + (2008)</a> explain this variation as arising due to differences |
| 17 | + between varieties in the timing of subject raising, using a split |
| 18 | + phi-probe. This paper revises their approach, using the feature |
| 19 | + gluttony mechanism for Agree developed in <a |
| 20 | + href="https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/004224">Coon & Keine |
| 21 | + (2020)</a>, and a split phi-probe in which person probing precedes |
| 22 | + number probing. Within this framework, the observed variation |
| 23 | + can be captured by allowing variability two independent |
| 24 | + parameters: the timing of EPP subject raising, and the visibility |
| 25 | + of a number feature on dative DPs. The proposed mechanism |
| 26 | + describes the variation, including predicting the observed |
| 27 | + optional agreement in certain cases that previous literature had |
| 28 | + struggled to account for, and makes additional predictions about |
| 29 | + the differences between varieties in cases of syncretism within |
| 30 | + the verbal paradigm. An investigation into these predictions |
| 31 | + should allow this already well-studied area of Icelandic grammar |
| 32 | + to continue to be a useful test-case for crosslinguistic |
| 33 | + assumptions about the mechanism of Agree, and the status of dative |
| 34 | + arguments.}, |
| 35 | + address = {Somerville, Mass., USA}, |
| 36 | + author = {Jacob Louis Hoover}, |
| 37 | + booktitle = {Proceedings of the 38th West Coast Conference on Formal |
| 38 | + Linguistics}, |
| 39 | + editor = {Daniel Reisinger and Rachel Soo}, |
| 40 | + handout = {http://doi.org/10.14288/1.0389856}, |
| 41 | + pdf = {wccfl2020-icelandic_gluttony-proceedings.pdf}, |
| 42 | + publisher = {Cascadilla Proceedings Project}, |
| 43 | + pubstate = {forthcoming}, |
| 44 | + title = {Accounting for variation in number agreement in Icelandic |
| 45 | + dative--nominative constructions}, |
| 46 | + year = {2020}} |
23 | 47 |
|
24 |
| -@misc{hoover.j:2021, |
25 |
| - abstract = {Are pairs of words that tend to occur together also likely to stand in a linguistic dependency? This empirical question is motivated by a long history of literature in cognitive science, psycholinguistics, and NLP. In this work we contribute an extensive analysis of the relationship between linguistic dependencies and statistical dependence between words. Improving on previous work, we introduce the use of large pretrained language models to compute contextualized estimates of the pointwise mutual information between words (CPMI). For multiple models and languages, we extract dependency trees which maximize CPMI, and compare to gold standard linguistic dependencies. Overall, we find that CPMI dependencies achieve an unlabelled undirected attachment score of at most $\approx 0.5$. While far above chance, and consistently above a non-contextualized PMI baseline, this score is generally comparable to a simple baseline formed by connecting adjacent words. We analyze which kinds of linguistic dependencies are best captured in CPMI dependencies, and also find marked differences between the estimates of the large pretrained language models, illustrating how their different training schemes affect the type of dependencies they capture.}, |
26 |
| - archiveprefix = {arXiv}, |
27 |
| - author = {Jacob Louis Hoover and Alessandro Sordoni and Wenyu Du and Timothy J. O'Donnell}, |
28 |
| - date-modified = {2021-09-10 10:36:49 -0400}, |
29 |
| - eprint = {2104.08685}, |
30 |
| - note = {Accepted to EMNLP2021}, |
31 |
| - primaryclass = {cs.CL}, |
32 |
| - title = {Linguistic Dependencies and Statistical Dependence}, |
33 |
| - year = {2021}} |
| 48 | +@inproceedings{hoover-etal-2021-linguistic, |
| 49 | + title = "Linguistic Dependencies and Statistical Dependence", |
| 50 | + author = "Hoover, Jacob Louis and |
| 51 | + Du, Wenyu and |
| 52 | + Sordoni, Alessandro and |
| 53 | + O{'}Donnell, Timothy J.", |
| 54 | + booktitle = "Proceedings of the 2021 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing", |
| 55 | + month = nov, |
| 56 | + year = "2021", |
| 57 | + address = "Online and Punta Cana, Dominican Republic", |
| 58 | + publisher = "Association for Computational Linguistics", |
| 59 | + url = "https://aclanthology.org/2021.emnlp-main.234", |
| 60 | + pages = "2941--2963", |
| 61 | + abstract = "Are pairs of words that tend to occur together also likely to stand in a linguistic dependency? This empirical question is motivated by a long history of literature in cognitive science, psycholinguistics, and NLP. In this work we contribute an extensive analysis of the relationship between linguistic dependencies and statistical dependence between words. Improving on previous work, we introduce the use of large pretrained language models to compute contextualized estimates of the pointwise mutual information between words (CPMI). For multiple models and languages, we extract dependency trees which maximize CPMI, and compare to gold standard linguistic dependencies. Overall, we find that CPMI dependencies achieve an unlabelled undirected attachment score of at most $\approx 0.5$. While far above chance, and consistently above a non-contextualized PMI baseline, this score is generally comparable to a simple baseline formed by connecting adjacent words. We analyze which kinds of linguistic dependencies are best captured in CPMI dependencies, and also find marked differences between the estimates of the large pretrained language models, illustrating how their different training schemes affect the type of dependencies they capture.", |
| 62 | + poster = {2021.10.11.EMNLP.poster.pdf}, |
| 63 | + code = {https://github.com/mcqll/cpmi-dependencies}, |
| 64 | + slides = {2021.10.11.EMNLP.talk-slides.pdf}, |
| 65 | +} |
0 commit comments